Wind turbine syndrome likely caused by scare campaigns – research

wind turbine syndrome

Anti-wind farm activists around the world have created a silent bogeymanthey claim can cause everything from sickness and headaches to herpes, kidney damage and cancers.

This “infrasound” exists at frequencies too low for the human ear to detect but is present almost everywhere from offices and roadsides to waves tumbling on ocean beaches. These low frequencies can crawl menacingly from the back of your kitchen fridge or from your heart beating.

Despite the ubiquitous nature of infrasound, anti-wind farm groups such as Australia’s Waubra Foundation like people to think that it’s only inaudible infrasound from wind turbines which might send residents to their sick beds.

But two new studies suggest the cause of health complaints by people living near wind farms could in fact be down to the scare campaign of the anti-wind groups and reports about such scares in the media.

The first study Can Expectations Produce Symptoms From Infrasound Associated With Wind Turbines? was published earlier this month in Health Psychology – a journal of the American Psychological Association.

The researchers from the University of Auckland in New Zealand wanted to find out if simply exposing people to warnings that turbines might make you ill was enough to cause them to report typical symptoms such as headaches and nausea.

Using 54 people, the researchers showed half the group five minutes of footage of people complaining that wind farms had made them ill. Some of the footage was taken from this Australian Broadcasting Corporation report (watch it here) into “Waubra disease” where residents were filmed complaining about a wind farm at Waubra in Victoria. Footage was also taken from this CTV Network report from Canada about a wind farm in Ontario.

This group was called the “high expectancy group” because the information they were given had led them to expect they might experience certain symptoms if exposed to infrasound. The other half of the group was shown interviews with experts stating that the science showed infrasound could not directly cause health problems.

The researchers then told each person they were going to be exposed to two 10-minute periods of infrasound in a special acoustic room when, in fact, for one of those periods they would be exposed to no sound at all, or “sham infrasound” as the researchers describe it.  So what happened?

The response from the “high expectancy” group was to report that the “infrasound” had caused them to experience more symptoms which were more intense. This was the case whether they were exposed to sham infrasound or genuine infrasound. The report explains that “the number of symptoms reported and the intensity of the symptom experienced during listening sessions were not affected by exposure to infrasound but were influenced by expectancy group allocation.”

In the low expectancy group, the infrasound and sham infrasound had little to no effect. In other words, the study found that if a person is told that wind turbines will make them ill then they are likely to report symptoms, regardless of whether they are exposed to infrasound or not.

Clearly, this points the finger at anti-wind farm campaigns as a potential cause of people’s symptoms, rather than “infrasound” from turbines. The research added: “The importance of findings in this study is that symptom expectations were created by viewing TV material readily available on the Internet, indicating the potential for such expectations to be created outside of the laboratory in real-world settings.”

Writing about her research on The Conversation, lead author Fiona Crichton says

The findings indicate that negative health information readily available to people living in the vicinity of wind farms has the potential to create symptom expectations, providing a possible pathway for symptoms attributed to operating wind turbines. This may have wide-reaching implications. If symptom expectations are the root cause of symptom reporting, answering calls to increase minimum wind-farm set back distances is likely to do little to assuage health complaints.

Reading some news reports (such as those offered by The Australian newspaper’s environment editor Graham Lloyd or anti-wind activist and UK anti-wind columnistJames Delingpole) and material from anti-wind farm groups, it might seem that health complaints are common among people living near turbines.

But an as yet unpublished study (and therefore not peer-reviewed) just released by Simon Chapman, the Professor of Public Health at the University of Sydney, suggests only a tiny proportion of people living near turbines do actually complain and, when they do, the complaints coincide with campaigning from anti-wind groups.

Chapman looked at health complaints made by residents living within 5 kilometres of all 49 wind farms operating in Australia between 1993 and 2012. After reviewing media reports, public inquiries and complaints to wind companies themselves, Chapman found evidence of only 120 individuals having actually complained – representing about 1 in 272 people living near wind farms.

But significantly, Chapman found that 81 of those 120 residents were living beside just five wind farms “which have been heavily targeted by anti wind farm groups”. What’s more, some 82 per cent of all the complaints had occured since 2009 when Chapman says anti-wind farm groups began to push the health scare as part of their opposition to turbines.

Some 31 of the 49 wind farms studied had never been subjected to a complaint either about noise or health.

“The 31 farms with no histories of complaints, and which today have some 21,530 residents within 5km of their turbines have operated for a cumulative total of 256 years,” says Chapman’s report. In Chapman’s research, he says that anxiety among residents increases as media reports spread the stories of health concerns and as researchers start investigating.

One down side to this research is, of course, that it tells anti-wind farm groups that by concentrating on unproven health concerns, their campaigns can illicit a steady flow of complaints and negative sentiment from communities.

This article was first published on DeSmogBlog. Reproduced with permission of the author.

Comments

7 responses to “Wind turbine syndrome likely caused by scare campaigns – research”

  1. Dave Johnson Avatar
    Dave Johnson

    With all this scary talk of infrasound perhaps we need to be careful about standing too close or for too long in the vicinity of certain politicians.

    After all there are probably all sorts of dangerous sound waves coming from their heads as their pea-sized brains rattle around in their nearly-empty skulls.

    To solve this problem, I propose a 2 km setback for all such politicians – straight down a mine shaft.

    Not funny? Well, neither is climate change.

  2. ian Avatar
    ian

    very funny!

  3. Chris Fraser Avatar
    Chris Fraser

    Chapman also raised the old chestnut of fatal bat and bird-killing turbine blades. Which is to say the toll on the bird population is too high for turbines to be allowed to exist alongside them.

    I only wish. I would place a big turbine right next to the local imported pest indian mynah population – because they breed like lice and eat everything …

  4. George Papadopoulos Avatar
    George Papadopoulos

    In real life, people around wind farms, whether they believe it or not are exposed to low frequency noise most days of the year but the complaints and timing of symptoms are generally consistent (why didn’t the Aukland researchers examine this phenomenon instead?).

    One might as well waste money and time by repeating the same experiment on a beach side and try convince people that natural infrasound is going to make them annoyed and stop them from sleeping.

  5. George Papadopoulos Avatar
    George Papadopoulos

    With reference to Simon Chapman’s study two peculiar aspects of his logic follow:

    “Companies were explicitly asked to not send details of any private complaints which could identify those complaining, unless these complaints had been made public by the complainants.”

    AND

    “Nearly all those who complained did not seek anonymity, being named in media reports or not electing to have their parliamentary submissions anonymised. However, we have chosen not to list their names in this report.”

    When dealing with the alleged culprit – the wind developer, why would the investigator trust the integrity of such individuals to self-assess data for inclusion into their study? And yet the concern to preserve anonymity is redundant by the fact that details on people on public were nevertheless suppressed. A little odd…

    I think Simon Chapman could have focussed his energy on understanding why a demoralised and dehumanised remote population only start complaining of their misery once outsiders encourage them to do so. The answer is rather obvious.

  6. john anstee Avatar
    john anstee

    I presume you mean ” elicit a steady flow of complaints “

    1. George Papadopoulos Avatar
      George Papadopoulos

      A “steady flow of complaints” would suggest a constant and presenting issue, as opposed to an imagined “nocebo” that lasts no longer that a few weeks perhaps.

Get up to 3 quotes from pre-vetted solar (and battery) installers.